Repressed Memory Validity

“As an undergraduate student in psychology, I was taught that multiple personalities were a very rare and bizarre disorder. That is all that I was taught on ... It soon became apparent that what I had been taught was simply not true. Not only was I meeting people with multiplicity; these individuals entering my life were normal human beings with much to offer. They were simply people who had endured more than their share of pain in this life and were struggling to make sense of it.”

― Deborah Bray Haddock, The Dissociative Identity Disorder Sourcebook

Accuracy of Recovered Memories


Image: "Light My Fire" by Botgirl Questi

CC BY-NC 2.0


It is difficult to learn of repressed memories without learning of the controversy surrounding their existence and validity. However, many discussions about repressed memories rely on outdated arguments and assumptions. It is true that some researchers doubt that traumatic memories can be completely repressed outside of a few rare cases (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018a), but this does not mean that they deny that traumatic memories can be temporarily forgotten and later more or less accurately remembered (Connolly & Read, 2007; Lindsay & Read, 1995; Loftus, 1993; Patihis & Pendergrast, 2018b). Many trauma survivors report having had no conscious access to memories of their trauma for a period of time (DePrince et al., 2012; Herman & Harvey, 1997), and many more report that they were temporarily unable to recall specific details about their trauma or specific traumatic instances (Beere, 2009; Herman & Harvey, 1997; Nijenhuis, Hart, & Steele, 2010). For reputable researchers of trauma, there is no debate over if genuine traumatic memories can be (or appear to be) repressed and then recovered. The main question is how often this occurs, and the second major question is what can increase the risk of traumatic memories being false. Other questions of interest are the mechanisms by which traumatic memories can be made temporarily inaccessible (addressed here), who recovers traumatic memories, what contexts are traumatic memories recovered under, and what types of trauma do traumatic memories often involve. All of these questions are important to understanding not just if recovered memories are a real phenomenon but also if the reasons that people doubt the validity of recovered memories are justified.


Who recovers memories of trauma, and what triggers these memories?

One reason that the public has such a negative view of recovered memories is the stereotypical image of those who recover memories, an image that is encouraged by organizations such as the False Memory Syndrome Foundation. The stereotypical individual with recovered memories is a suggestible woman who has significant emotional pathology (Birrell & Freyd, 2004) and who recovered memories of Satanic ritual abuse or other extreme sexual traumas through therapeutic practices of questionable validity, such as hypnosis. Additionally, this process of memory recovery is often framed as occurring during the so-called Satanic ritual abuse panic of the 1980s and subsequent memory wars of the 1990s (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018a). However, there is a large gap between the stereotype and the reality.


4% of all Americans and 11.3% of those in therapy report having recovered memories of child abuse at some point during their lives (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018a). In one community sample of 505 adults, 72% had experienced trauma, and 23% had temporarily not had conscious access to traumatic memories (Elliot, 1997). Men and women are equally likely to report having recovered memories of abuse, and there was only a moderate increase in reports of recovered memories during the 1990s compared to surrounding decades. Memory recovery can happen over the course of any type of therapy (e.g., attachment therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, marriage counseling). Around one-third of individuals recover memories outside of therapy, one-third within therapy, and one-third both inside and outside of therapy sessions (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018a). The same is true in the UK, where one-third of therapy clients who recovered memories did so prior to entering therapy, and less than one-fourth of cases described recovered memories that only emerged after the use of memory recovery techniques (Andrews, Brewin, Ochera, & Morton, 1999). In contrast, one study found that 68% of those with recovered memories sought therapy because of the emerging memories (Herman and Harvey, 1997). 


Most often (in 48% of cases), the return of traumatic memories is triggered by reminders of the trauma, such as returning to the neighborhood in which the trauma occurred or the death of an abuser. Life milestones or crises are another common trigger (relevant in 40% of cases) (Herman and Harvey, 1997). Other common triggers of memory recovery include seeing media that reminds one of their trauma, experiencing a similar trauma, or conversations with family (Elliot, 1997). Only 20% of cases involve an altered state of consciousness, such as hypnosis or dreaming (Herman and Harvey, 1997), and an even smaller percentage of recovered memory cases are associated with guided imagery (9%), social contagion within a survivor support group (7.4%), or dream interpretation (2.5%). Most memories are recovered through flashbacks (39.3%) (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018a) which first occur at home (Chu et al., 1999).


What types of trauma do recovered memories typically involve?

Despite the perception of recovered memories often involving satanic ritual abuse or extreme forms of abuse, only a tiny minority (2% to 5%) of individuals who report recovered memories report having recovered memories of ritual abuse. More prevalent are emotional abuse (74%), physical abuse (51%), sexual abuse (42 to 64.5%), and neglect (22%), with other possible traumas including medical procedures and witnessing a death (Andrews et al., 1999; Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018). Out of 690 therapy patients studied in the UK, only one case of memory recovery was reported to involve alien abduction, and none involved past life events. Memory recovery techniques did not affect the type of traumatic memory recovered (Andrews et al., 1999). The majority of recovered memories are believable to those who recover them; 92.6% of those who recovered memories in therapy report still believing that these memories are accurate years or decades later (Patihis and Pendergrast, 2018).


Contrary to what the main talking points about the recovered memories controversy would suggest, recovered memories were not originally discussed in the context of child abuse, and they have not always been met with such high levels of suspicion. Dissociative amnesia is well established as a potential result of war trauma; many of the first known cases of “shell shock” were assumed to follow concussions because of their association with a period of unremembered activity or a loss of previous autobiographical memory (Cuplin 1931; see also Brown & van der Hart, 1998; DePrince et al., 2012; Elliot, 1997; Sargant, W. & Slater). Some Holocaust survivors have reported amnesia for some or all of the traumas that they experienced (see Brown & van der Hart, 1998; van der Hart & Brom, 2000). Adult interpersonal traumas, accidents, and natural disasters have also been associated with dissociative amnesia (Brown & van der Hart, 1998; Elliot, 1997; Herman & Harvey, 1997). One of the first described cases of dissociative amnesia was Pierre Janet’s patient Irene, who was amnesiac to the death of her mother (Culpin, 1931; Brown & van der Hart, 1998; van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1991).


What evidence is there for the validity of recovered memories of abuse?

Unfortunately, recovered memories of abuse, particularly sexual abuse, are met with increased skepticism. The False Memory Syndrome Foundation was created to argue that severe childhood trauma is always remembered (Cheit, 1998; Pope, 1998), and similar views have been expressed by professionals such as Ofshe, Holmes, Gardner, and, more inconsistently, Loftus (Cheit, 1998; Gleaves, 1996; Herman & Harvey, 1997). This, of course, does not fit the data. Between 19% (Loftus, Polensky, & Fullilove, 1994) and 59% (Breire & Conte, 1993) of trauma survivors report having had completely inaccessible trauma memories for some amount of time. Clinical samples range from 33% to 37% for partial amnesia and 27% to 47% for complete amnesia, national samples range from 30% to 52% for some degree of amnesia, and one study of Japanese students found an amnesia rate of 68%. When looking specifically at survivors of childhood sexual abuse, samples of college students can return figures as low as 14% to as high as 38% (DePrince et al., 2012). Loftus et al. (1994) found that 12% of survivors of childhood sexual abuse report partial amnesia. For survivors of any type of crime who are seeking therapy, 17% to 36% report partial or moderate amnesia, and 16% to 28% report complete or severe amnesia (Herman and Harvey, 1997; Herman and Shatzow, 1987).


Of course, the major question is not how many people report recovered memories of abuse but instead how many people report accurate recovered memories of abuse. Here, the data is still in agreement. A large minority of individuals with confirmed cases of abuse report partial or complete amnesia for the abuse. Of individuals with confirmed histories of sexual abuse, reports of complete amnesia range from 10.8% (Goodman et al., 2003) to 47% (Herman & Harvey, 1997). 16% of female survivors of medically-documented sexual abuse studied by Williams (1994) reported a period of amnesia for their abuse, and their interviews were overall “remarkably consistent” and did not differ from the interviews of survivors with continuously accessible memories. Factors that make survivors of court-documented sexual abuse less likely to report the abuse include the abuse having occurred before age 5, reduced maternal support, and dissociation (Goodman et al., 2003).


Additionally, many individuals with recovered trauma memories can find corroboration for their memories. In one study, 57% of cases involving partial amnesia for trauma and 36% involving complete amnesia for trauma were corroborated; examples of corroborating evidence included another individual reporting being abused by the same abuser (52%), someone else confirming the abuse (43%), the abuser confessing (13%), or official (12%), court (9%), or medical (6%) records confirming the abuse. 34% of cases with corroboration involved multiple types of corroboration (Andrews et al., 1999). Corroboration rates are not significantly different between survivors who have continuously had access to their trauma memories and those who have had amnesia for their trauma. These rates range from 46% for survivors who have never had traumatic amnesia and 36% for those who have (Herman and Harvey, 1997) to almost three-fourths of all survivors regardless of whether or not the survivor has experienced traumatic amnesia (Herman and Shatzow, 1987). In Chu et al. (1999), 89% of the 19 women who attempted to find corroboration for their previously completely inaccessible memories of childhood sexual abuse were able to do so, as were 93% of the 14 women who sought corroboration of their recovered memories of physical abuse. Stoler (2001) found higher rates of corroboration for temporarily inaccessible (86%) compared to continuously accessible (46%) memories, which was explained as an artifact of those with recovered abuse memories being more likely to seek external confirmation of their memories. Many examples of corroborated recovered memories can be found at the Recovered Memory Project (Cheit, 1998; www.recoveredmemory.org). That said, the context in which the memories were recovered does matter. A study by Geraerts et al. (2007) found that 45% of survivors with continuous memories, 37% of survivors with temporarily inaccessible memories, and 0% of those who had recovered memories through suggestive therapy were able to obtain corroboration for their memories.


Why are some recovered memories more trustworthy than others?

Most trauma therapy is not suggestive. Although new details or memories related to trauma may emerge over the course of therapy, most trauma therapists do not actively encourage this. In general, trauma therapy focuses on systematic exposure and desensitization to already accessible traumatic memories (DePrince et al., 2012). However, in a minority of cases, therapy may rely heavily on so-called “memory recovery techniques” such as hypnosis, and this may create traumatic “memories” where there were none before (Chu et al., 1999; Loftus, 1993; Lindsay & Read, 1995; Patihis & Pendergrast, 2018a). Additionally, even genuine trauma memories can be distorted by therapeutic, social, or media influences (Loftus, 1993; Strange & Takarangi, 2015). When misleading or incorrect information is repeated when questioning an individual about their memories (for example, if a therapist or legal investigator repeatedly fishes for specific answers), 15% to 30% of individuals are vulnerable to incorporating this false information into their memories. This is true even for individuals with generally good memories and even when the event in question was stressful or potentially traumatic (Patihis & Pendergrast, 2018a). Entirely false memories of trauma are possible, and using hypnosis, guided imagery, or dream interpretation to aid recall can increase the risk of creating false traumatic memories. Internalizing the traumatic histories of close friends, support group members, or media figures is also a risk for some individuals. It is thought that individuals falsely incorporate details or entire narratives provided by others due to a failure in source monitoring; instead of being able to recognize the information as something external, the individual retrieves it as (part of) their own traumatic memory (Chu, Frey, Ganzel, & Matthews, 1999; Lindsay & Read, 1995; Loftus, 1993; Patihis & Pendergrast, 2018a). That said, the core features of a memory are less likely to change due to external influence than are specific details (Herman & Harvey, 1997; Veldhuis & Freyd, 1999).


Overall Implications

The official stance of the American Psychological Association (n.d.) and of many trauma researchers (Connolly & Read, 2007; DePrince et al., 2012) is that it is possible both for an individual to temporarily be unable to access genuine traumatic memories and for an individual to have memories of trauma, recovered or otherwise, that are partially or entirely incorrect. Even many researchers who were previously known for arguing against the possibility of accurate recovered memories now acknowledge the abundance of evidence that traumatic memories can be temporarily partially or completely inaccessible and later be more or less accurately recalled (DePrince et al., 2012). That said, research is ongoing. Establishing how often traumatic memories, recovered or otherwise, are accurate is important for determining the best therapeutic practices and legal policies. Additionally, statistics about recovered memories in general say nothing about the accuracy of individual cases, which is what matters most to those who recover traumatic memories and to those who support them. Confirming what factors make memories less likely to be accurate and what signs might indicate that a memory is less accurate are important to judging individual cases in the absence of external corroboration.

 

 



American Psychological Association (n.d.). Memories of childhood abuse. Retreived from https://www.apa.org/topics/trauma/memories

Birrell, P. J. & Freyd, J. J. (2004). Speaking for ourselves: Unmasking the hidden agenda of the false memory controversy. [Review of the book Relational remembering: Rethinking the memory wars, by S. Campbell]. Ethics and Behavior, 14(1), 89-92.

Beere, D. B. (2009). The self-system as mechanism for the dissociative disorders: An extension of the perceptual theory of dissociation. In P. F. Dell & J. A. O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: DSM-V and beyond. New York, NY: Routledge.

Briere, J. & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6(1), 21-31.

Brown, P. & van der Hart, O. (1998). Memories of sexual abuse: Janet’s critique of Freud, a balanced approach. Psychological Reports, 82, 1027-1043.

Cheit, R. E. (1998). Consider this, skeptics of recovered memory. Ethics & Behavior, 8(2), 141-160.

Chu, J. A., Frey, L. M., Ganzel, B. L., & Matthews, J. A. (1999). Memories of childhood abuse: Dissociation, amnesia, and corroboration. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(5), 749-755.

Connolly, D. A. & Read, D. (2007). Canadian criminal court reports of historic child sexual abuse: Factors associated with delayed prosecution and reported repression. In M. E. Pipe, M. E. Lamb, Y. Orbach, & A. C. Cederborg (Eds.), Child sexual abuse: Disclosure, delay, and denial (pp. 195-216). New York, NY: Routledge.

Culpin, M. (1931). Recent advances in the study of the psychoneuroses. Philadelphia, PA: P. Blakiston’s Son.

DePrince, A. P, Brown, L. S., Cheit, R. E., Freyd, J. J., Gold, S. N., Pezdek, K., & Quina, K (2012). Motivated forgetting and misremembering: Perspectives from Betrayal Trauma Theory.  In R. F. Belli (Ed.), True and false recovered memories: Toward a reconciliation of the debate (pp. 193-243). New York: Springer.

Elliot, D. M. (1997). Traumatic events: Prevalence and delayed recall in the general population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(5), 811-820.

Geraerts, E., Schooler, J. W., Merckelbach, H., Jelicic, M., Hauer, B. J. A., & Ambadar, Z. (2007). The reality of recovered memories. Psychological Science, 18(7), 564-568.

Gleaves, D. H. (1996). The evidence for "repression": An examination of Holmes (1990) and the implications for the recovered memory controversy. Journal of Child Sex Abuse, 5(1).

Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Edelstein, R. S., Alexander, K. W., Redlich, A. D., … Jones, D. P. H. (2003). A prospective study of memory for child sexual abuse: New findings relevant to the repressed memory controversy. Psychological Science, 14(2), 113-118.

Herman, J. L. & Harvey, M. R. (1997). Adult memories of childhood trauma: A naturalistic clinical study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(4), 557-571.

Herman, J. E. & Shatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of memories of childhood sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4(1), 1-14.

Lindsay, D. S. & Read, J. D. (1995). “Memory work” and recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: Scientific evidence and public, professional, and personal issues. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1, 846-908.

Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48, 518-537.

Loftus, E. F., Polensky, S., & Fullilove, M. T. (1994). Memories of childhood sexual abuse: Remembering and repressing. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 67-84.

Nijenhuis, E.R.S., Van der Hart, O., & Steele, K. (2010). Trauma-related structural dissociation of the personality. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 52(1), 1-23.

Patihis, L. & Pendergrast, M. H. (2018a). Reports of recovered memories of abuse in therapy in a large age-representative U.S. national sample: Therapy type and decade comparisons. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 3-21.

Patihis, L. & Pendergrast, M. H. (2018b). Reports of recovered memories in therapy, informed consent, and generalizability: Response to commentaries. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 32-36.

Pope, K. S. (1998). Pseudoscience, cross-examination, and scientific evidence in the recovered memory controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 4(4), 1160-1181.

Sargant, W. & Slater, E. (1941). Amnesiac syndromes in war. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 34, 757-764.

Strange, D. & Takarangi, M. K. T. (2015). Memory distortion for traumatic events: The role of mental imagery. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6(27).

Stoler, L. R. (2001). Recovered and continuous memories of childhood sexual abuse: A quantitative and qualitative analysis. (Doctoral Dissertation, University of Rhode Island). Dissertation Abstracts International, 61(10-B), 5582.

van der Hart, O. & Brom, D. (2000). When the victim forgets. In A. Y. Shalev, R. Yehuda, & A. C. McFarlane (Eds.), International handbook of human response to trauma (pp. 233-248). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

van der Kolk, B. A. & van der Hart, O. (1991). The intrusive past: The flexibility of memory and the engraving of trauma. American Imago, 48(4), 425-454.

Veldhuis, C. B. & Freyd, J. J. (1999). Groomed for silence, groomed for betrayal. In M. Rivera (Ed.), Fragment by fragment: Feminist Perspectives on memory and child sexual abuse (pp. 253-282). Charlottetown, PEI Canada: Gynergy Books.

Williams, L. M. (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(6), 1167–1176.